From: Rafal Smigrodzki (rms2g@virginia.edu)
Date: Tue Sep 03 2002 - 13:48:55 MDT
Lee Corbin wrote:
Rafal, like gts (though less well-informed), everything that I
have ever heard confines emotions to the limbic system. Probably
also like him (fat chance he's a she) I don't feel like perusing
all the many pointers in all your previous posts.
Could you give any specific one of them that indicates a role
of the cortex in subjective experience of emotion?
### Better, I'll give you 23 (so few only because I didn't feel like quoting
all). Basically, the cortex is intimately involved in emotional responses,
as evidenced by lesion studies, fMRI studies, as well as brain structural
and (even) single cell recordings. To claim that the cortex is a "cold
calculating organ" (as gts wrote, or something to that effect) is plainly
silly. There is no methodology yet to clearly point to the location of a
subjective experience, or even define subjectivity in physicalist terms, but
most definitely the cortex cannot be excluded from consideration.
One thought experiment - try to imagine a pair of amygdala, kept alive in a
tank, with some electric stimulation delivered to it - if indeed the
amygdala were the sole seat of emotion, these disembodied structures would
feel as strongly as you and me. I regard this idea to be as preposterous as
the idea that chunks of the brain kept alive and firing without exchanging
information would have the subjective experience of being a person. The mind
is in the brain, not in any single part of it. It takes the amygdala *and*
the cortex to produce emotion in a human (and I don't know about gators).
Any subjective experience that an intact brain has, is a function of high
level information processing, with exchanges between many areas weaving a
coherent network, as in the information exchanged between the amygdala and
prefrontal cortex.
Here are the refs.
I sign off from this thread.
Rafal
----------------------------------------------------------------------------
---- 1. Calder AJ. Keane J. Manes F. Antoun N. Young AW. Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury. Nature Neuroscience. 3(11):1077-8, 2000 Nov. HEALTH SCIENCES LIBRARY OWNS - CLICK ON LIBRARY HOLDINGS UI: 11036262 Huntington's disease can particularly affect people's recognition of disgust from facial expressions, and functional neuroimaging research has demonstrated that facial expressions of disgust consistently engage different brain areas (insula and putamen) than other facial expressions. However, it is not known whether these particular brain areas process only facial signals of disgust or disgust signals from multiple modalities. Here we describe evidence, from a patient with insula and putamen damage, for a neural system for recognizing social signals of disgust from multiple modalities. Medline Identifier 20493464 Institution MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, 15 Chaucer Road, Cambridge CB2 2EF, UK. andy.calder@mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk 2. Blair RJ. Cipolotti L. Impaired social response reversal. A case of 'acquired sociopathy'. Brain. 123 ( Pt 6):1122-41, 2000 Jun. HSL OWNS PRINT & ONLINE hslnet.med.virginia.edu/ejrnl.html UI: 10825352 In this study, we report a patient (J.S.) who, following trauma to the right frontal region, including the orbitofrontal cortex, presented with 'acquired sociopathy'. His behaviour was notably aberrant and marked by high levels of aggression and a callous disregard for others. A series of experimental investigations were conducted to address the cognitive dysfunction that might underpin his profoundly aberrant behaviour. His performance was contrasted with that of a second patient (C.L.A.), who also presented with a grave dysexecutive syndrome but no socially aberrant behaviour, and five inmates of Wormwood Scrubs prison with developmental psychopathy. While J.S. showed no reversal learning impairment, he presented with severe difficulty in emotional expression recognition, autonomic responding and social cognition. Unlike the comparison populations, J.S. showed impairment in: the recognition of, and autonomic responding to, angry and disgusted expressions; attributing the emotions of fear, anger and embarrassment to story protagonists; and the identification of violations of social behaviour. The findings are discussed with reference to models regarding the role of the orbitofrontal cortex in the control of aggression. It is suggested that J.S.'s impairment is due to a reduced ability to generate expectations of others' negative emotional reactions, in particular anger. In healthy individuals, these representations act to suppress behaviour that is inappropriate in specific social contexts. Moreover, it is proposed that the orbitofrontal cortex may be implicated specifically either in the generation of these expectations or the use of these expectations to suppress inappropriate behaviour. Medline Identifier 20340248 Institution Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Department of Psychology, University College London, UK. j.blair@ucl.ac.uk 3. Adolphs R. Damasio H. Tranel D. Cooper G. Damasio AR. A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping. Journal of Neuroscience. 20(7):2683-90, 2000 Apr 1. HSL OWNS PRINT & ONLINE hslnet.med.virginia.edu/ejrnl.html UI: 10729349 Although lesion and functional imaging studies have broadly implicated the right hemisphere in the recognition of emotion, neither the underlying processes nor the precise anatomical correlates are well understood. We addressed these two issues in a quantitative study of 108 subjects with focal brain lesions, using three different tasks that assessed the recognition and naming of six basic emotions from facial expressions. Lesions were analyzed as a function of task performance by coregistration in a common brain space, and statistical analyses of their joint volumetric density revealed specific regions in which damage was significantly associated with impairment. We show that recognizing emotions from visually presented facial expressions requires right somatosensory-related cortices. The findings are consistent with the idea that we recognize another individual's emotional state by internally generating somatosensory representations that simulate how the other individual would feel when displaying a certain facial expression. Follow-up experiments revealed that conceptual knowledge and knowledge of the name of the emotion draw on neuroanatomically separable systems. Right somatosensory-related cortices thus constitute an additional critical component that functions together with structures such as the amygdala and right visual cortices in retrieving socially relevant information from faces. Medline Identifier 20195730 Institution Department of Neurology, Division of Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Iowa College of Medicine, Iowa City, Iowa 52242, USA. ralph-adolphs@uiowa.edu 4. Mandal MK. Borod JC. Asthana HS. Mohanty A. Mohanty S. Koff E. Effects of lesion variables and emotion type on the perception of facial emotion. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease. 187(10):603-9, 1999 Oct. HEALTH SCIENCES LIBRARY OWNS - CLICK ON LIBRARY HOLDINGS UI: 10535653 The purpose of this study was to consider the effects of valence, motoric direction (i.e., approach/withdrawal), and arousal on the perception of facial emotion in patients with unilateral cortical lesions. We also examined the influence of lesion side, site, and size on emotional perception. Subjects were 30 right-hemisphere-damaged (RHD) and 30 left-hemisphere-damaged (LHD) male patients with focal lesions restricted primarily to the frontal, temporal, or parietal lobe. Patient groups were comparable on demographic and clinical neurological variables. Subjects were tested for their ability to match photographs of four facial emotional expressions: happiness, sadness, fear, and anger. Overall, RHD patients were significantly more impaired than LHD patients in perceiving facial emotion. Lesion side, but not site, was associated with motoric direction and valence dimensions. RHD patients had specific deficits relative to LHD patients in processing negative and withdrawal emotions; there were no group differences for positive/approach emotions. Lesion size was not significantly correlated with accuracy of emotional perception. Medline Identifier 20004311 Institution Department of Humanities & Social Sciences, Indian Institute of Technology, Kharagpur, India. 5. Zahn TP. Grafman J. Tranel D. Frontal lobe lesions and electrodermal activity: effects of significance. Neuropsychologia. 37(11):1227-41, 1999 Oct. HSL OWNS PRINT & ONLINE hslnet.med.virginia.edu/ejrnl.html UI: 10530723 Several studies have shown that cortical damage, especially to the right hemisphere and to frontal lobes, may attenuate skin conductance responses selectively to psychologically significant stimuli. We tested this hypothesis in 32 patients with frontal lesions, verified by computer assisted tomography and magnetic resonance imaging, and 45 healthy controls. Patients and controls were given a protocol which included a rest period, a series of innocuous tones, and a reaction time task. Patients were given a second protocol in which they viewed slides with positive and negative emotional content and neutral slides. Results showed attenuated electrodermal activity (EDA) during task instructions and smaller skin conductance responses to reaction-time stimuli in patients compared to controls but few differences under passive conditions or in orienting responses to simple tones. Patients with lateral prefrontal and paraventricular lesions were especially low in EDA in the reaction time task, and those with right and bilateral lesions in the cingulate gyrus and/or frontal operculum had attenuated EDA in both protocols. We conclude that the effects of certain frontal lesions are on the psychological response to significance which is indexed by EDA rather than directly on EDA per se. Medline Identifier 99458306 Institution Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD 20892-9005, USA. 6. Paradiso S. Chemerinski E. Yazici KM. Tartaro A. Robinson RG. Frontal lobe syndrome reassessed: comparison of patients with lateral or medial frontal brain damage. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 67(5):664-7, 1999 Nov. HSL OWNS PRINT & ONLINE hslnet.med.virginia.edu/ejrnl.html UI: 10519877 Examination of mood and behaviour changes after frontal damage may contribute to understanding the functional role of distinct prefrontal areas in depression and anxiety. Depression and anxiety disorders, symptoms, and behaviour were compared in eight patients with single lateral and eight patients with single medial frontal lesions matched for age, sex, race, education, socioeconomic status, side, and aetiology of lesion 2 weeks and 3 months after brain injury. DSM IV major depressive and generalised anxiety disorders were more frequent in patients with lateral compared with medial lesions at 2 weeks but not at 3 months. At 3 months, however, patients with lateral damage showed greater severity of depressive symptoms, and greater impairment in both activities of daily living and social functioning. At initial evaluation depressed mood and slowness were more frequent, whereas at 3 months slowness, lack of energy, and social unease were more frequent in the lateral than the medial group. Patients with lateral lesions showed greater reduction of emotion and motivation (apathy) during both examinations. Medial frontal injury may fail to produce emotional dysregulation or may inhibit experience of mood changes, anxiety, or apathy. Lateral prefrontal damage may disrupt mood regulation and drive while leaving intact the ability to experience (negative) emotions. Medline Identifier 99451146 Institution Department of Psychiatry, University of Iowa Iowa City, IA 52241, USA. sergio-paradiso@uiowa.edu 7. Montreys CR. Borod JC. A preliminary evaluation of emotional experience and expression following unilateral brain damage. International Journal of Neuroscience. 96(3-4):269-83, 1998 Dec. HEALTH SCIENCES LIBRARY OWNS - CLICK ON LIBRARY HOLDINGS UI: 10069626 In this preliminary study, hemispheric specialization for the experience and expression of emotion was investigated. Subjects were right-brain-damaged (RBD), left-brain-damaged (LBD), and normal control (NC) right-handed adults, carefully matched on demographic and neurological variables. Facial expressions were videotaped while subjects described recollected emotional and nonemotional experiences. Expressions were later rated by trained judges for emotional intensity and category accuracy. To examine experience, subjects evaluated the intensity and accuracy with which they had produced their monologues. RBDs produced less intense facial expressions and reported less intense emotional experiences than LBDs and NCs. LBDs rated themselves as producing expressions with less accuracy than did RBDs and NCs. These findings are consistent with research that supports the right-hemisphere hypothesis for emotion. In addition, judges' ratings and subjects' self-reports of emotional intensity were positively correlated for normal but not for brain-damaged subjects. Medline Identifier 99167156 Institution Queens College and the Graduate School, City University of New York, Flushing 11367, USA. 8. Grafman J. Schwab K. Warden D. Pridgen A. Brown HR. Salazar AM. Frontal lobe injuries, violence, and aggression: a report of the Vietnam Head Injury Study. Neurology. 46(5):1231-8, 1996 May. HSL OWNS PRINT & ONLINE hslnet.med.virginia.edu/ejrnl.html UI: 8628458 Knowledge stored in the human prefrontal cortex may exert control over more primitive behavioral reactions to environmental provocation. Therefore, following frontal lobe lesions, patients are more likely to use physical intimidation or verbal threats in potential or actual confrontational situations. To test this hypothesis, we examined the relationship between frontal lobe lesions and the presence of aggressive and violent behavior. Fifty-seven normal controls and 279 veterans, matched for age, education, and time in Vietnam, who had suffered penetrating head injuries during their service in Vietnam, were studied. Family observations and self-reports were collected using scales and questionnaires that assessed a range of aggressive and violent attitudes and behavior. Two Aggression/Violence Scale scores, based on observer ratings, were constructed. The results indicated that patients with frontal ventromedial lesions consistently demonstrated Aggression/Violence Scale scores significantly higher than controls and patients with lesions in other brain areas. Higher Aggression/Violence Scale scores were generally associated with verbal confrontations rather than physical assaults, which were less frequently reported. The presence of aggressive and violent behaviors was not associated with the total size of the lesion nor whether the patient had seizures, but was associated with a disruption of family activities. These findings support the hypothesis that ventromedial frontal lobe lesions increase the risk of aggressive and violent behavior. Medline Identifier 96212551 Institution Cognitive Neuroscience Section, NIH/NINDS/MNB, Bethesda, MD 20892-1440, USA. 9. Mochizuki H. Saito H. Mesial frontal lobe syndromes: correlations between neurological deficits and radiological localizations. [Review] [18 refs] Tohoku Journal of Experimental Medicine. 161 Suppl:231-9, 1990 Aug. HEALTH SCIENCES LIBRARY OWNS - CLICK ON LIBRARY HOLDINGS UI: 2082501 In order to detect the functional subsets of the mesial frontal lobe, 26 patients with lesions involving mesial frontal lobe were investigated. The results were summarized as follows; (1) Hemiparesis predominantly in lower extremity was common motor deficit in patients with mesial frontal motor cortices. Four patients with supplementary motor damage sparing motor cortex showed long lasting hemiparesis. (2) Verbal adynamia was common symptom due to left supplementary motor and superior prefrontal damage. Transient mutism was found in two patients with superior prefrontal damage without the involvement of supplementary motor cortex. (3) Emotional disturbance characterized by depressed motivation was also common. No hemispheric laterality could be detected in these patients. (4) Dementia and memory disturbance related to the pole and anterior area of superior prefrontal damage. (5) Transient urinary incontinence was presented only by right superior prefrontal damage, and permanent incontinence related with bilateral damage. (6) Akinetic mutism was related with bilateral large damage including cingulate. (7) Motor neglect and dressing apraxia possibly due to programming disturbance related to lesions in right hemisphere. [References: 18] Medline Identifier 91188451 Institution Department of Neurology, Tohoku University School of Medicine, Sendai. 10. Grafman J. Vance SC. Weingartner H. Salazar AM. Amin D. The effects of lateralized frontal lesions on mood regulation. Brain. 109 ( Pt 6):1127-48, 1986 Dec. HSL OWNS PRINT & ONLINE hslnet.med.virginia.edu/ejrnl.html UI: 3790971 A group of Vietnam veterans with penetrating brain wounds to the orbitofrontal, dorsofrontal, and nonfrontal cortex were compared with a stratified control group of self-report and observed measures of mood state and cognition. In particular, hypotheses regarding the regulation of anxiety by frontal cortical mechanisms were evaluated. Results indicated that patients with right orbitofrontal lesions were prone to abnormally increased 'edginess'/anxiety and depression, whereas patients with left dorsofrontal lesions were prone to abnormally increased anger/hostility. A working model of mood state regulation is presented which represents the thesis that mood sensations are subject to numerous cognitive and biological influences that result in a variety of expressions of a particular mood disorder. Medline Identifier 87077400 Citation Manager: Display, Print, Save, or Email Citations Citations Fields Citation Format Action Selected Citations All in this set (1-10) and/or Range: Citation (Title,Author,Source) Citation + Abstract Citation + Abstract + Subject Headings Complete Reference Ovid BRS/Tagged Reprint/Medlars Brief (Titles) Display Direct Export ---------------------------------------------------------------------------- ---- Include Search History Sort Keys Primary: -Unique IdentifierMedline IdentifierRecord OwnerAuthorsTitleJournal NameYear of PublicationLanguageCountry of PublicationPublication TypeJournal SubsetEntry DateAscendingDescending Secondary: -Unique IdentifierMedline IdentifierRecord OwnerAuthorsTitleJournal NameYear of PublicationLanguageCountry of PublicationPublication TypeJournal SubsetEntry DateAscendingDescending ---------------------------------------------------------------------------- ---- Copyright (c) 2000-2002 Ovid Technologies, Inc. Version: rel5.0.0, SourceID 1.6100.1.189 ---------------------------------------------------------------------------- ---- Results of your search: from 11 keep 6-7, 9, 16, 22, 25, 33... Citations displayed: 1-10 of 13 Go to Record: Citation Manager Help Logoff 1. Berthoz S. Artiges E. Van De Moortele PF. Poline JB. Rouquette S. Consoli SM. Martinot JL. Effect of impaired recognition and expression of emotions on frontocingulate cortices: an fMRI study of men with alexithymia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 159(6):961-7, 2002 Jun. HSL OWNS PRINT & ONLINE hslnet.med.virginia.edu/ejrnl.html UI: 12042184 OBJECTIVE: Although the brain areas involved in emotional response and in the recognition of others' emotions have been reported, the neural bases of individual differences in affective style remain to be elucidated. Alexithymia, i.e., impairment of the ability to identify and communicate one's emotional state, influences how emotions are regulated. Alexithymia has been hypothesized to involve anterior cingulate dysfunction. Therefore, the authors searched for differential cerebral regional activation in response to emotional stimuli in subjects with alexithymia. METHOD: Two groups of eight men each were selected from 437 healthy subjects on the basis of high or low scores on the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), the authors compared the two groups for their regional cerebral activation in response to the presentation of pictures with validated positive or negative arousal capabilities. RESULTS: Men with alexithymia demonstrated less cerebral activation in the left mediofrontal-paracingulate cortex in response to highly negative stimuli and more activation in the anterior cingulate, mediofrontal cortex, and middle frontal gyrus in response to highly positive stimuli than men without alexithymia. CONCLUSIONS: These findings provide direct evidence that alexithymia, a personality trait playing a role in affect regulation, is linked with differences in anterior cingulate and mediofrontal activity during emotional stimuli processing. Medline Identifier 22037441 Institution Equipe de Recherche ERM 0205, Imagerie Cerebrale en Psychiatrie, Institut National de la Sante et de la Recherche Medicale, Service Hospitalier Frederic Joliot-Commissariat a l'Energie Atomique, Orsay, France. 2. Adolphs R. Neural systems for recognizing emotion. [Review] [99 refs] Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 12(2):169-77, 2002 Apr. HSL OWNS PRINT & ONLINE hslnet.med.virginia.edu/ejrnl.html UI: 12015233 Recognition of emotion draws on a distributed set of structures that include the occipitotemporal neocortex, amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex and right frontoparietal cortices. Recognition of fear may draw especially on the amygdala and the detection of disgust may rely on the insula and basal ganglia. Two important mechanisms for recognition of emotions are the construction of a simulation of the observed emotion in the perceiver, and the modulation of sensory cortices via top-down influences. [References: 99] Medline Identifier 22010036 Institution Division of Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Neurology, 200 Hawkins Drive, University of Iowa College of Medicine, 52242, USA. ralph-adolphs@uiowa.edu 3. Zysset S. Huber O. Ferstl E. von Cramon DY. The anterior frontomedian cortex and evaluative judgment: an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 15(4):983-91, 2002 Apr. NOT OWNED BY HS LIBRARY - CLICK ON LIBRARY HOLDINGS UI: 11906238 This study investigated the neuronal basis of evaluative judgment. Judgments can be defined as the assessment of an external or internal stimulus on an internal scale and they are fundamental for decision-making and other cognitive processes. Evaluative judgments (I like George W. Bush: yes/no) are a special type of judgment, in which the internal scale is related to the person's value system (preferences, norms, aesthetic values, etc.). We used functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine brain activation during the performance of evaluative judgments as opposed to episodic and semantic memory retrieval. Evaluative judgment produced significant activation in the anterior frontomedian cortex (BA 10/9), the inferior precuneus (BA 23/31), and the left inferior prefrontal cortex (BA 45/47). The results show a functional dissociation between the activations in the anterior frontomedian cortex and in the inferior precuneus. The latter was mainly activated by episodic retrieval processes, supporting its function as a multimodal association area that integrates the different aspects of retrieved and newly presented information. In contrast, the anterior frontomedian cortex was mainly involved in evaluative judgments, supporting its role in self-referential processes and in the self-initiation of cognitive processes. (C)2002 Elsevier Science (USA). Medline Identifier 21904925 Institution Max-Planck-Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, D-04303 Leipzig, Germany. 4. Gray JR. Braver TS. Raichle ME. Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 99(6):4115-20, 2002 Mar 19. HSL OWNS PRINT & ONLINE hslnet.med.virginia.edu/ejrnl.html UI: 11904454 We used functional MRI to test the hypothesis that emotional states can selectively influence cognition-related neural activity in lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC), as evidence for an integration of emotion and cognition. Participants (n = 14) watched short videos intended to induce emotional states (pleasant/approach related, unpleasant/withdrawal related, or neutral). After each video, the participants were scanned while performing a 3-back working memory task having either words or faces as stimuli. Task-related neural activity in bilateral PFC showed a predicted pattern: an Emotion x Stimulus crossover interaction, with no main effects, with activity predicting task performance. This highly specific result indicates that emotion and higher cognition can be truly integrated, i.e., at some point of processing, functional specialization is lost, and emotion and cognition conjointly and equally contribute to the control of thought and behavior. Other regions in lateral PFC showed hemispheric specialization for emotion and for stimuli separately, consistent with a hierarchical and hemisphere-based mechanism of integration. Medline Identifier 21902095 Institution Department of Psychology, Washington University, One Brookings Drive, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA. 5. Moll J. de Oliveira-Souza R. Eslinger PJ. Bramati IE. Mourao-Miranda J. Andreiuolo PA. Pessoa L. The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions. Journal of Neuroscience. 22(7):2730-6, 2002 Apr 1. HSL OWNS PRINT & ONLINE hslnet.med.virginia.edu/ejrnl.html UI: 11923438 Humans are endowed with a natural sense of fairness that permeates social perceptions and interactions. This moral stance is so ubiquitous that we may not notice it as a fundamental component of daily decision making and in the workings of many legal, political, and social systems. Emotion plays a pivotal role in moral experience by assigning human values to events, objects, and actions. Although the brain correlates of basic emotions have been explored, the neural organization of "moral emotions" in the human brain remains poorly understood. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging and a passive visual task, we show that both basic and moral emotions activate the amygdala, thalamus, and upper midbrain. The orbital and medial prefrontal cortex and the superior temporal sulcus are also recruited by viewing scenes evocative of moral emotions. Our results indicate that the orbital and medial sectors of the prefrontal cortex and the superior temporal sulcus region, which are critical regions for social behavior and perception, play a central role in moral appraisals. We suggest that the automatic tagging of ordinary social events with moral values may be an important mechanism for implicit social behaviors in humans. Medline Identifier 21921027 Institution Neuroimaging and Behavioral Neurology Group, Hospitais D'Or and LABS, RJ, 22281-081, Brazil. jmoll@neuroimage.com 6. Davidson RJ. Anxiety and affective style: role of prefrontal cortex and amygdala. [Review] [152 refs] Biological Psychiatry. 51(1):68-80, 2002 Jan 1. HSL OWNS PRINT & ONLINE hslnet.med.virginia.edu/ejrnl.html UI: 11801232 This article reviews the modern literature on two key aspects of the central circuitry of emotion: the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the amygdala. There are several different functional divisions of the PFC, including the dorsolateral, ventromedial, and orbital sectors. Each of these regions plays some role in affective processing that shares the feature of representing affect in the absence of immediate rewards and punishments as well as in different aspects of emotional regulation. The amygdala appears to be crucial for the learning of new stimulus-threat contingencies and also appears to be important in the expression of cue-specific fear. Individual differences in both tonic activation and phasic reactivity in this circuit play an important role in governing different aspects of anxiety. Emphasis is placed on affective chronometry, or the time course of emotional responding, as a key attribute of individual differences in propensity for anxiety that is regulated by this circuitry. [References: 152] Medline Identifier 21659270 Institution Laboratory for Affective Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1202 West Johnson Street, Madison, WI 53706, USA. 7. Shin LM. Whalen PJ. Pitman RK. Bush G. Macklin ML. Lasko NB. Orr SP. McInerney SC. Rauch SL. An fMRI study of anterior cingulate function in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 50(12):932-42, 2001 Dec 15. HSL OWNS PRINT & ONLINE hslnet.med.virginia.edu/ejrnl.html UI: 11750889 BACKGROUND: Several recent neuroimaging studies have provided data consistent with functional abnormalities in anterior cingulate cortex in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). In our study, we implemented a cognitive activation paradigm to test the functional integrity of anterior cingulate cortex in PTSD. METHODS: Eight Vietnam combat veterans with PTSD (PTSD Group) and eight Vietnam combat veterans without PTSD (non-PTSD Group) underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) while performing the Emotional Counting Stroop. In separate conditions, subjects counted the number of combat-related (Combat), generally negative (General Negative), and neutral (Neutral) words presented on a screen and pressed a button indicating their response. RESULTS: In the Combat versus General Negative comparison, the non-PTSD group exhibited significant fMRI blood oxygenation level-dependent signal increases in rostral anterior cingulate cortex, but the PTSD group did not. CONCLUSIONS: These findings suggest a diminished response in rostral anterior cingulate cortex in the presence of emotionally relevant stimuli in PTSD. We speculate that diminished recruitment of this region in PTSD may, in part, mediate symptoms such as distress and arousal upon exposure to reminders of trauma. Medline Identifier 21621488 Institution Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, MA 02155, USA. 8. Houde O. Zago L. Crivello F. Moutier S. Pineau A. Mazoyer B. Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Access to deductive logic depends on a right ventromedial prefrontal area devoted to emotion and feeling: evidence from a training paradigm. Neuroimage. 14(6):1486-92, 2001 Dec. NOT OWNED BY HS LIBRARY - CLICK ON LIBRARY HOLDINGS UI: 11707105 Does the human capacity for access to deductive logic depend on emotion and feeling? With positron emission tomography, we compared the brain networks recruited by two groups of subjects who were either able or not able to shift from errors to logical responses in a deductive reasoning task. They were scanned twice while performing the same task, before and after a training session. The error-to-logical shift occurred in a group that underwent logicoemotional training but not in the other group, trained in logic only-a "cold" kind of training. The intergroup comparison pointed out that access to deductive logic involved a right ventromedial prefrontal area known to be devoted to emotion and feeling. Copyright 2001 Academic Press. Medline Identifier 21564451 Institution Groupe d'Imagerie Neurofonctionnelle, UMR 6095, CNRS, CEA, Universite de Caen, 14000 Caen, France. houde@cyceron.fr 9. Blood AJ. Zatorre RJ. Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 98(20):11818-23, 2001 Sep 25. HSL OWNS PRINT & ONLINE hslnet.med.virginia.edu/ejrnl.html UI: 11573015 We used positron emission tomography to study neural mechanisms underlying intensely pleasant emotional responses to music. Cerebral blood flow changes were measured in response to subject-selected music that elicited the highly pleasurable experience of "shivers-down-the-spine" or "chills." Subjective reports of chills were accompanied by changes in heart rate, electromyogram, and respiration. As intensity of these chills increased, cerebral blood flow increases and decreases were observed in brain regions thought to be involved in reward/motivation, emotion, and arousal, including ventral striatum, midbrain, amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex, and ventral medial prefrontal cortex. These brain structures are known to be active in response to other euphoria-inducing stimuli, such as food, sex, and drugs of abuse. This finding links music with biologically relevant, survival-related stimuli via their common recruitment of brain circuitry involved in pleasure and reward. Medline Identifier 21457361 Institution Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada. ablood@nmr.mgh.harvard.edu 10. Wright CI. Fischer H. Whalen PJ. McInerney SC. Shin LM. Rauch SL. Differential prefrontal cortex and amygdala habituation to repeatedly presented emotional stimuli. NeuroReport. 12(2):379-83, 2001 Feb 12,. HEALTH SCIENCES LIBRARY OWNS - CLICK ON LIBRARY HOLDINGS UI: 11209954 Repeated presentations of emotional facial expressions were used to assess habituation in the human brain using fMRI. Significant fMRI signal decrement was present in the left dorsolateral prefrontal and premotor cortex, and right amygdala. Within the left prefrontal cortex greater habituation to happy vs fearful stimuli was evident, suggesting devotion of sustained neural resources for processing of threat vs safety signals. In the amygdala, significantly greater habituation was observed on the right compared to the left. In contrast, the left amygdala was significantly more activated than the right to the contrast of fear vs happy. We speculate that the right amygdala is part of a dynamic emotional stimulus detection system, while the left is specialized for sustained stimulus evaluations. Medline Identifier 21077273 Institution Nuclear Magnetic Resonance Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Charlestown 02129, USA. Citation Manager: Display, Print, Save, or Email Citations Citations Fields Citation Format Action Selected Citations All on this page All in this set (1-13) and/or Range: Citation (Title,Author,Source) Citation + Abstract Citation + Abstract + Subject Headings Complete Reference Ovid BRS/Tagged Reprint/Medlars Brief (Titles) Display Direct Export ---------------------------------------------------------------------------- ---- Include Search History Sort Keys Primary: -Unique IdentifierMedline IdentifierRecord OwnerAuthorsTitleJournal NameYear of PublicationLanguageCountry of PublicationPublication TypeJournal SubsetEntry DateAscendingDescending Secondary: -Unique IdentifierMedline IdentifierRecord OwnerAuthorsTitleJournal NameYear of PublicationLanguageCountry of PublicationPublication TypeJournal SubsetEntry DateAscendingDescending ---------------------------------------------------------------------------- ---- Copyright (c) 2000-2002 Ovid Technologies, Inc. Version: rel5.0.0, SourceID 1.6100.1.189
This archive was generated by hypermail 2.1.5 : Sat Nov 02 2002 - 09:16:40 MST