summaryrefslogtreecommitdiff
path: root/web/ufHelp/Q9-3.html
blob: 1deb17e04f0012431daedacb593ac26282479658 (plain)
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
<head>
<script>
	function loadPercentile(name, k5, k50, k95) {
			var args = 
					{
							"caption": "Loaded our interpretation of " + name + "\'s probability.",
							"Q9-3.y50.0k5.0": k5,
							"Q9-3.y50.0k50.0": k50,
							"Q9-3.y50.0k95.0": k95
					};
			top.loadData(args);
	}
</script>
</head>
<body>
<P CLASS="western" STYLE="margin-bottom: 0in;">Keeping in mind your
given probabilities for genome sequencing costs, how quickly will we learn what
genes tend to make people into good researchers?</P>

<P CLASS="western" STYLE="margin-bottom: 0in">Phrased differently:
how much more productive will individuals created using
multi-generational <I>in vitro</I> embryo selection be when they grow
up, given our knowledge at the time, and given that the technology is
permitted and used? (If you think it won't be, that will be addressed
in the following question.) Consider a productivity of 1 to be
equivalent to that of the average (mean) non-embryo-selected
scientist.</P>
<P>We acknowledge that children selected for traits
contributing to research ability may be a minority of children
selected for any reason, but assume that at least some group will
still be selected for traits such as working memory, curiosity, and so on,
and state how much more productive they'll be if they
choose to go into research.</P>

<UL>
	<LI><P CLASS="western" STYLE="margin-bottom: 0in">
	<B>Claim:</B>
	Above a certain level of cognitive
	ability, further increases have minimal value. For instance, the famous
	California Terman study of high-IQ youth failed to include two
	future Nobel laureates in physics because they were somewhat below
	its IQ cutoff for entry.<BR>
	<B>Implication:</B> Selection for
	general cognitive function is unlikely to have great effects on
	scientific output.
	<input type="button" onclick="loadPercentile('Gladwell', 0.01, 0.11, 0.21);" value="Load distribution"</input><BR>
	<B>Sources:</B>
	Gladwell, Malcolm. <I>Outliers</I><SPAN STYLE="font-style: normal">.
	New York: Little, Brown, and Company, 2008.</SPAN></P>
	</LI>
	<LI><P CLASS="western" STYLE="margin-bottom: 0in">
	<B>Claim:</B>
	The personality trait
	of openness to experience, which involves active imagination,
	aesthetic sensitivity, attentiveness to inner feelings, preference
	for variety, and intellectual curiosity, which makes important
	contributions to research ability, is known to be heritable, and
	therefore could be selected for in multi-generational <I>in
	vitro</I> embryo
	selection procedures. Identical and fraternal twins reared apart
	correlated .43 and .23, respectively, for Openness, while identical
	and fraternal twins reared together correlated .51 and .14. This was
	the highest correlation of any of the five major dimensions of
	personality.<BR>
	<B>Implication:</B>
	To the extent that psychological Openness correlates to research
	ability, it can be selected for fairly effectively.
	<input type="button" onclick="loadPercentile('Jang et al.', 0.01, 0.2826, 0.5552);" value="Load distribution"</input><BR>
	<B>Source:</B>
	Jang et al. &quot;Heritability of the Big Five Personality Dimensions
	and Their Facts: A Twin Study&quot;. <I>Journal
	of Personality</I> 64:3, September 1996.<BR>&lt;<U><A TARGET="_blank" HREF="http://www.psych.umn.edu/courses/fall06/yoonh/psy3135/articles/Jang%20et%20al_1996.pdf">http://www.psych.umn.edu/courses/fall06/yoonh/psy3135/articles/Jang%20et%20al_1996.pdf</A></U>&gt;</P>
	<LI><P CLASS="western" STYLE="margin-bottom: 0in">
	<B>Claim:</B> The <A TARGET="_blank" HREF="http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Heritability">heritability</A> 
	of IQ can be estimated at anywhere between 0.4 and 0.8, depending on
	the data used, e.g. twin studies, correlations between siblings, and
	adoption studies.<BR>
	<B>Implication:</B> 
	IQ, which contributes to research ability, is at least somewhat
	heritable, so extensive embryo selection could increase it
	significantly. However, the impact
	of such an increase would depend on the degree to which science is
	limited by the supply of highly intelligent talent
	and how to estimate the effects of embryos with more
	favorable genetic predispositions to cognitive ability than any past
	humans have enjoyed.
	<input type="button" onclick="loadPercentile('Neisser', 0.01, 0.3010, 0.59);" value="Load distribution"</input><BR>
	<B>Source:</B>
	Neisser et al.
	&quot;Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns&quot;. <I>American
	Psychologist</I>. Vol	51(2), Feb 1996, 77-101.<BR>&lt;<U><A TARGET="_blank" HREF="http://psychnet.apa.org/?fa=main.doiLanding&amp;doi=10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77">http://psychnet.apa.org/?fa=main.doiLanding&amp;doi=10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77</A></U>&gt;</P>
	<LI><P CLASS="western" STYLE="margin-bottom: 0in">
	<B>Claim:</B>
	&quot;The
	research, led by Robert Plomin of the Institute of Psychiatry in
	London, identified six genes that were strongly associated with high
	or low intelligence, but even the most powerful of these accounted
	for just 0.4 per cent of the variation in intelligence between
	individuals. The six together accounted for about 1 per cent of the
	variation in intelligence. Dozens of previous studies on twins and
	adopted children have established that about half of the variation
	in intelligence is down to environment.&quot; (2007)<BR>
	<B>Implication:</B>
	Though there are correlations between abilities useful for research
	and genetic variations, any individual genetic variation explains
	only a very small portion of differences between people in cognitive
	ability. Very large studies sequencing the genomes of hundreds of
	thousands or millions of individuals may be required to identify a
	substantial fraction of relevant variation. Some variants will be so
	rare that statistics cannot be assembled, forcing researchers to
	infer their effects from functional and other information, taking
	years after large-scale genome sequencing data become available.<BR>
	<B>Sources:</B>
	Coghlan, Andy.
	&quot;'Intelligence genes' reveal their complexity.&quot;
	<I>New&nbsp;Scientist </I>29 Nov. 2007.
	&lt;<U><A TARGET="_blank" HREF="http://www.newscientist.com/channel/being-human/mg19626324.100-intelligence-genes-reveal-their-complexity.html">http://www.newscientist.com/channel/being-human/mg19626324.100-intelligence-genes-reveal-their-complexity.html</A></U>&gt;.<BR>
	L M Butcher, O S P Davis, I W Craig, and R Plomin. &quot;Genome-wide
	quantitative trait locus association scan of general cognitive
	ability using pooled DNA and 500k single nucleotide polymorphism
	microarrays.&quot; <I>Genes, Brain, and  Behavior</I> 
	7 (2008): 435-446. 
&lt;<U><A TARGET="_blank" HREF="http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2408663">http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2408663</A></U>&gt;
	</P>
	<LI><P CLASS="western" STYLE="margin-bottom: 0in">
	<B>Claim:</B>
	&quot;Talent-search participants
	(286 males, 94 females) scoring in the top 0.01% on
	cognitive-ability measures were identified before age 13 and tracked
	over 20 years. Their creative, occupational, and life
	accomplishments are compared with those of graduate students (299
	males, 287 females) enrolled in top-ranked U.S. mathematics,
	engineering, and physical science programs in 1992 and tracked over
	10 years. By their mid-30s, the two groups achieved comparable and
	exceptional success (e.g., securing top tenure-track positions) and
	reported high and commensurate career and life satisfaction. [...]
	Individuals identified solely on the basis of one very high SAT
	score before the age of 13 achieved occupational success comparable
	to that of individuals attending world-class mathematics, science,
	and engineering graduate training programs.&quot;<BR>
	<B>Implication:</B>
	If there is a substantial correlation between genes and research
	ability, and this genetic material can be used to select embryos
	that will grow into adults with this exceptional ability, then the
	practical impact could be quite large. The ability multiplier for
	embryo-selected children could be as high as 100 or even
	1,000.
	<input type="button" onclick="loadPercentile('Lubinski', 0.5, 2, 3.5);" value="Load distribution"</input><BR>
	<B>Source: </B>
	Lubinski, David, Camilla P. Benbow, Rose Mary Webb, and April
	Bleske-Rechek. &quot;Tracking Exceptional Human Capital Over Two
	Decades.&quot; <I>Psychological
	Science</I> 17 (2006): 194-99. Retrieved 9 Aug. 2008.
	&lt; <U><A TARGET="_blank" HREF="http://www.vanderbilt.edu/Peabody/SMPY/PsychScience2006.pdf">http://www.vanderbilt.edu/Peabody/SMPY/PsychScience2006.pdf</A></U>&gt;.</P>
	<LI><P CLASS="western" STYLE="margin-bottom: 0in">
	<B>Claim:</B>
	&quot;Within the United States, the regional differences in intellectual
	accomplishment have been nothing shy of astonishing...The population
	of Texas has exceeded that of New England for the last hundred years
	by a factor of 3 or 4...But a visit to <I>Who's
	Who</I>
	establishes that Texans have achieved virtually nothing in the
	sciences or philosophy (though there have been notable achievements
	in literature, music, and the arts in recent decades).&quot;<BR>
	<B>Implication:</B>
	Research output critically depends on a culture of science:
	increasing abilities may not be channeled into advancing the state
	of scientific knowledge.<BR>
	<B>Source:</B> Nisbett, Richard.
	<I>Intelligence and How
	to Get It</I>. New York:
	Norton. 2009.<BR>
	</P>
</UL>

</body>